Institute for Christian
Teaching
Education Department of
Seventh-day Adventists
AND UNIVERSITIES: SUGGESTED
GUIDELINES
by
Beverly J. Rumble
Editor, Journal of
Adventist Education
General Conference of
Seventh-day Adventists
Silver Spring, Maryland
Prepared for the
International Faith and
Learning Seminar
held at
Newbold College, Bracknell,
Berkshire, England
June 1994
207-94 Institute for
Christian Teaching
12501 Old Columbia Pike
Silver Spring, MD 20904 USA
The term "human experimentation" often
brings to mind horrifying abuses of human rights, such as experiments in Nazi
concentration camps. Other, less famous
examples include the following:
In the late
1800s E. Franenkel inoculated the eyes of terminally ill infants with gonorrhea
cultures.[1]
Early in the 20th
century Richard P. Strong (later professor of tropical medicine at Harvard)
injected plague germs into death-row inmates in the Philippines.[2]
In the Tuskegee
(Alabama) syphilis experiment that stretched over 40 years, researchers
withheld treatment from 399 poor black men so that they could study the
physical effects of untreated venereal disease.[3]
In 1966, Henry
Beecher, a Harvard Medical School professor, exposed more recent abuses in
human experimentation. His "roll
of dishonor" included withholding of penicillin from uninformed servicemen
with streptococcal infections, a number of whom contracted rheumatic fever;
feeding of live hepatitis viruses to residents of a state institution for the
retarded to study the disease and attempt to develop a vaccine; the injection
of live cancer cells into elderly and senile hospitalized patients without
telling them the nature of the cells; and a number of other shocking abuses.
Beecher maintained that "unethical or questionably ethical procedures are
not uncommon" among researchers, and that the rights of human subjects
were widely disregarded.[4]
In order to
investigate whether side effects such as nervousness and depression could be
caused by oral contraceptives, Goldzieher gave dummy pills to 76 women who
sought treatment in a San Antonio, Texas, clinic to prevent further
pregnancies. None of the women was told
that she was participating in research or receiving placebos. Most of the
experimental subjects were poor Mexican-Americans with several children.[5]
In at least 31
separate experiments between the mid-1940s and the 1970s the U.S. Government
exposed nearly 700 people to radioactive substances. These included an
experiment in which Manhattan Project scientists, the designers of the first
atomic bomb, injected 18 people with "relatively massive quantities of
bomb-grade plutonium" to see how much of the toxic substance their bodies
would retain.[6]
The above examples highlight abuses in medical
experiment. But some investigations by
social scientists have been scarcely less questionable. For example:
Zillman and
Bryant randomly assigned 80 male and female undergraduates to watch various
amounts of heterosexual pornography of a six-week period. The students were
then asked to estimate the percentage of U.S. adults performing certain sexual
acts, and to recommend a prison term for a rapist described in a newspaper
article.[7]
To evaluate a
questionnaire that proported to test peoples' comprehension of moral
principles, a team of social scientists proposed to administer it to teenagers
at a male juvenile delinquency rehabilitation center, and then tempt them to
lie or steal.[8]
Posing as
fellow believers, researchers covertly studied a small flying saucer cult whose
members were waiting for the end of the world. The ratio of
researcher-believers was so high, however, that their participation wronged
those studied not only by lying to them but also by providing false
"evidence" to reinforce their beliefs (and at the same time, altering
the phenomena under investigation).[9]
Other
studies have attempted to find a genetic link to criminal behavior or
intelligence,[10] or have
exposed subjects to psychoactive drugs without their knowledge.
Professors and students at
Seventh-day Adventist healthcare training institutions, as well as at our other
colleges and universities sometimes conduct medical and social science
experiments and studies using human subjects.
In the United States, the federal government closely regulates medical
experimentation on humans; therefore, that area will not be the primary focus
of this paper, though some of the policies established for healthcare
institutions provide the basis of its suggestions for social science research.
Administrators would be well advised to use these guidelines in developing
policies for their institutions. A list
of helpful sources is included at the end of the paper.
Reasons of Experimenting on Human Subjects
As abhorrent as the above
experiments seem to us today, they appeared perfectly defensible and even
urgently necessary to the people who performed them. So we are forced to look
more closely at the reasons for using human beings in research.
In general, researchers
argue that hey are contributing to and extending human knowledge through
research, and that their studies advance health, science, and human welfare
(perhaps also providing the researchers with some renown in the annals of
science). An ethicist cementing on such
research said that these scientists seek to avoid the "menace of avoidable
ignorance."[11] In times of
national emergency, such as wartime, research seems a patriotic imperative, for
it provides knowledge that may help subdue enemy aggression or save the lives
of military personnel.
Researchers claim that they
need to use human subjects in their studies because they cannot achieve the
same results with simulations or animals. This is especially true in social
science research. In resisting external
controls on their research, they argue that freedom of inquiry is essential for
healthy research.[12]
Despite researchers'
commitment to the general good and to extending human knowledge, their
"omnivorous appetite"[13]
for scientific research, as ethicist Paul Ramsey puts it, can cause them to
lose sight of the importance of each individual subject. As Henry Beecher
pointed out.
"Any classification of human experimentation as
'for the good of society' is to be viewed with distaste, even alarm.
Undoubtedly all sound work has this as its ultimate aim, but such high-flown
expressions. . . have been used within recent memory as cover for outrageous
acts. . . There is no justification. . .for risking an injury for the benefit
of other people. . .such a rule would open the door wide to perversions of
practice, even such as were inflicted by Nazi doctors on concentration camp
prisoners. . .The individual must not be subordinated to the community."[14]
Although progress depends
heavily on science and technology, we no longer naively assume that this is the
way to the good life. Nuclear weapon,
widespread pollution and abuses of human beings in research like that cited
above have sensitized us to the potential for evilas well as goodthat science
and technology can achieve. Previously
undreamed-of capabilities for human beings to control birth and death, to
transplant human organs, even to manipulate genetic material, have highlighted
the need for external controls over researchers and medical personnel.
Exposes in scientific
journals and the popular press have revealed a stark conflict of interest
between researchers' ambitions and subjects' well being. As a result, in the U.S. federal regulations
have been passed requiring oversight and collective decision making.[15]
The areas that have
generated the most serious ethical problems are the design of the experiment,
choice of subjects, obtaining informed consent, balancing risk to the subject
with benefits to the researcher and society, and maintaining the participant's
dignity and privacy. These topics will be dealt with in greater detail later in
this paper.
Moral Implications
As Christians, we view the
scientific method differently from those who hold a naturalistic philosophy
about the origins of human beings. We believe that God designed the universe to
operate in an orderly way, although He may occasionally step outside of natural
processes to perform miracles.
Therefore, since the universe is orderly, and God made human beings
capable of rational thought, we can design experiments to explore the
mechanisms of the physical universe and even to study human behavior through
the social sciences. We can thereby discover some of the marvelous aspects of
God's creation, extend the boundaries of knowledge, and alleviate human
suffering.
However, our beliefs will
put certain constraints on the kinds of scientific research that we do. For
example, if we believe that human beings are created in the image of God, we
will treat humanity at every stage of life with respecteven miscarried
fetuses, human tissue, and frozen embryosin contrast to how we would act if we
believed that homo-sapiens are essentially no different from other creatures in
the natural world.
Human experimentation raises
a number of religious and ethical dilemmas.
Traditionally, such research occurred in the field of medicine, where
the physician was expected to be committed to the good of the individual
patient. The primary rule of medical morality was to do no harmbased on the
Hippocratic Oath and guidelines for medical ethics drawn up by the General
Assembly of the World Medical Association in 1949.[16]
The timing of the latter guidelines was doubtless inspired in part by the
Nuremberb trials, which revealed the flagrant abuses of Nazi researchers.
How do we balance the needs
of the individual with those of society? Should researchers be allowed to
induce or coerce individuals to subject themselves to certain risks for the
good of others? To clarify this, let us look at two extreme positions: The
first would subordinate the individual to the good of society. This would allow
medical and psychological experimentation on human beings without their consent
if the studies would benefit society. This represents the classical utilitarian
theory of choosing the greatest good for the greatest number. Thus, anytime a
researcher could claim that a procedure or experiment would benefit society, he
or she could justify overriding the rights of the individuals involved for each
person counts for only one.
At the opposite extreme,
absolute individualism acknowledges no significant relationship between the
individual and society, and asserts the primacy of the individual over the
group. Most ethicist acknowledge that at times the individual must be limited
by the needs of society, and that each person has an obligation, as part of a
community, to act in ways that benefit others.
Human beings do not exist as isolated atoms. They
are actually constituted by their relationshipsto the world, to their family,
to their fellow human beings, to the Church, and to God. It is important to
stress that these relationships are not extrinsic or spatial but intrinsic;
they belong to the very fabric of each person's being.[17]
Jesus' admonition to
"love your neighbor as yourself" ties together both respect for
persons and one's obligation to the larger community.
It may be helpful at this
point to divide moral obligations into two categoriesthose that are required
of all human beings, and others that are heroicabove and beyond the call of
duty. Some people would want to volunteer for experiments to help others,
despite the risk to themselves. But no one should be coerced into participating
in such studies. In every case, we must evaluate the balance between the risks
and potential benefits and give potential subjects enough information to allow
them to make an informed choice.
In recent times, Western
society has become more concerned about protecting the individual against
possible invasions of dignity, privacy, and freedom. Christian ethics has
always asserted that every person possesses certain inalienable rights, that
individuals are ends in themselves. They are never to be used merely as a means
to something elseno matter what their race or color, how well or poorly
endowed with talents, or how primitive or developed. Therefore, the individual
takes primacy over society. However much the good of the whole exceeds that of
any of its parts, and whatever duties each person owes to society, individuals
constitute the supreme value, and society exists only to promote the good of
its members.
"In view of people's
tendency to exploit their fellow human beings, the scriptural revelation of the
innate, inalienable dignity and value of the individual provides an indispensable
bulwark of freedom and growth."[18]
Christ's example and teachings and the admonitions of Old Testament prophets
provide a basis ethical framework for making decisions about how to treat
people, both in daily life and in experimentation.
Our Lord has taught us that the Decalogue is
centrally a statement of what love demands. But since justice is one of the
things that love enjoins, it is possible to distill from the Ten Commandments a
list, even though it be a partial list, of the rights and liberties men can
claim. . . This. . is only a partial list of the rights we tend to claim. . .
We can go to the Old Testament prophets and learn from, say, Hosea, that the
powerless have a right to be protected against the strong.[19]
Each human being is unique,
created in the image of God and redeemed at an infinite price. He or she
possesses the power to think and to do, according to Ellen White.[20]
This means that God places a high value on allowing each person to choose
freely what actions he or she will take. This principle should influence
researchers' choice of subjects and topics for investigation. They too are free to choose, but should keep
in mind Paul's admonition: "Brethren, ye have been called unto liberty;
only use not liberty for an occasion to the flesh, but by love serve one
another" (Galatians 5:13).
Research, optimally, should
consist of a "truly joint venture between two human beings working
together for the increase of human knowledge and the ability of human beings to
serve one another. From this perspective, the subject is a coparticipant in the
human quest for progress."[21]
This gives the subject a more active role, and requires the researcher to
respect his or her humanity and rights as a freewill agent. Therefore, as Hans
Jonas points out, the most highly motivated, the most highly educated, and the
least captive members of our communities would make the best research subjects.
Subjects with poorer knowledge, motivation, and decision-making freedoms (who
are consequently more readily available in terms of number and possible
manipulation) should be used more sparingly and reluctantly.[22]
Curran suggests as a criterion whether one would subject his or her own
children to the proposed experiments.[23]
Unfortunately, this has not been the usual method of choosing subjects
for experimentation. Those from lower socioeconomic classes, minorities,
prisoners, the gullible, and the mentally incompetent have borne an undue share
of the burden as subjects of experimentation.
To treat individuals
ethically means not only to respect their decisions and protect them from harm,
but also to actively attempt to ensure their well-being. This is encompassed in
the term beneficence, which
implies the following principles for research on human beings: "(1) do not
harm, and (2) maximize possible benefits and minimize possible harms."[24]
Although discovering what
will in fact benefit people may require exposing them to risk, forethought and
oversight are needed to assess when it is justified to seek certain benefits
despite the risks involved, and when the experiment should be forgone because
of the possible risks. Such risks may
involve psychological, physical, legal, social, or economic harm.
For the Christian
researcher, the principle of stewardship is also a relevant concern. "We
do not possess anything in the world, absolutely, not even our own [or other
people's] bodies; we hold things in trust for God, who created them, and are
bound, therefore, to use them only as He intends that they should be
used."[25]
Montemorelos University's
"Philosophy and Role of Research" statement expresses well the
concept of stewardship:
A consciousness of our stewardship of God's creation
prohibits the investment of time, ability, or economics resources in the search
of knowledge that may result in adverse effects for human life, or that involve
immoral elements or consequences. By the same token, this consciousness
motivates us to the diligent research of all practical possibilities toward the
common well-being of mankind.[26]
Christian principles should
be applied at every stage of researchplanning the study, choosing subjects,
informing participants of the risks and benefits, performing the experiment,
debriefing subjects, and guarding the privacy of subjectsas well as careful
analysis of the data, reporting of the study, and ethical use of the data after
the study has been completed.
Design of the Experiment of Study
David Rutstein points out
that "Attention must be given to the ways an experiment can be designed to
maintain its scientific validity, meet ethical requirements, and yet yield the
necessary new knowledge."[27]
The experiment's design goes straight to the basis questions asked by the
investigator. What problem does he or she seek to solve? What information does
he desire to obtain? Are the proposed methods and techniques consistent with
Christian ethics?
In the medical area, a
standard question is whether the experimentation is therapeutic, or conducted
only for its research value. Research is clearly non-therapeutic when it is
carried out solely to gain information that will benefit others, but which is
of no use to the patient.
An analysis of the design of
social science experiments should address the following questions:
Is it ethical
to ask people to perform the actions specified by the researcher?
Will the procedures
cause psychological injury or humiliation to the subjects?
Could any part
of the research cause irreversible changes in the subjects' personality or
moral values?
Do the
researcher's actions mislead subjects by lending support to false ideas or
prejudices?
Application of the principles underlying these
questions would clearly eliminate any proposals that require participants to
perform illegal or immoral acts, that expose them to pornography or depictions
of violence, or ask them to participate in psychological experiments that would
be demeaning to themselves or to others.
Potential conflicts of
interest and threats to researcher integrity also present ethical
dilemmas. Research sponsored by an
organization that expects a particular result (a tobacco company, for example),
whether or not it uses human subjects, should raise a red flag for researchers
who seek to do pure science and arrive at truth, unencumbered by any type of
coercion. Christian researchers will doubtless also want to engage in serious
reflection and prayer, perhaps seeking pastoral and ethical guidance, before
participating in research that may contribute to military weaponry or be used
to destroy or harm human beings or the natural world.
Choosing Subjects for Research
Methods for choosing
subjects may not be as obvious or ethically neutral as some researchers would
have us believe. Participant selection raises ethical issues on two levels:
social and individual. Researchers should not offer potentially beneficial research
only to certain categories of persons, or select only "undesirable"
persons for risky research. Social
justice requires that distinctions be drawn between classes of subjects. Those
who cannot give consent or who ought not to be further burdened, such as the
indigent, children, the institutionalized or mentally infirm, and prisoners
should be used only under certain carefully controlled conditions.
Conversely, a variety of
groups should be included in research, particularly if the anticipate benefits
of the studies might better their lives.
This would mean, for example, not testing drugs only on men, or
selecting only young, well-educated subjects for research on organ transplants.
Disclosure
Candor helps ensure the
integrity of the encounter between researcher and subject, and prevents the
researcher from exploiting and subordinating participants to the research
process. It demeans the subject's humanity to see him or her simply as a case
or a statistic, a mere representative of some class or category of persons.
As autonomous agents, human beings have the right to
control their own lives and to receive enough information to make informed
decision; therefore, researchers should share adequate facts to enable the
subjects to judge for themselves the balance between risk and benefit, and to
decide whether to participate in the study.
In general, the law imposes a strict duty of
disclosure, wherever an individual with a great deal to lose is exposed to a
risk or is asked to relinquish rights by someone with considerably greater
knowledge.[28]
Less than complete
disclosure deprives the subject of the opportunity to consciously and
deliberately render service in a crucial social enterprise, while greater
candor enables him or her to become a partner in the thrill of scientific
discovery and perhaps relief of human misery.
Therefore, each subject
should receive an explanation of the procedures to be followed and their
purpose, including (1) identification of any procedures that are experimental;
(2) a description of the discomforts and risks, as well as benefits that may
reasonably be expected; (3) disclosure of any appropriate alternative
procedures that might be advantageous; (4) an offer to explain any questions
about the procedures; (6) assurance that the subject is free to withdraw his or
her consent and to stop participating in the project at any time.[29]
Coercion
Subjects should participate
in research without coercion, after the risks and benefits of their
participation have been fully explained.
But there remains some question whether certain groups are capable of
making truly free choices, either because of their circumstances or the nature
of their relationship to the researchers.
If the explanations are
geared to their level of comprehension, even poorly educated persons can be
considered capable of participating freely in research. However, certain groups present ethical
difficulties because they cannot give informed consent or are susceptible to
coercion.
Payment for participation
may raise concern, if care is promised to indigent persons in exchange for
participating in experimentation, or if people "volunteer" for a
study because of financial need or their desire for some benefit, such as
reduced prison sentence.
Other who cannot give
consent, or who are incapable of understanding the nature of the proposed
experiment, constitute a particularly vulnerable group. This would include children, the underclass,
and the mentally incompetent.
Jesus' example is
instructive in this area. According to
Robert Mortimer, "The Son of God showed particular care and concern for
the fallen, the outcast, the weak and the despised."[30]
Likewise, researchers should treat with special regard those in vulnerable
groups.
John Fletcher, a Christian
ethicist who has devoted much study to the practical aspects of informed
consent, suggests that several other factors can affect the autonomy of
subjects: whether they are ill or dependent on the researcher for medical care,
the circumstances surrounding the institution, and the desire to please the
investigation.[31]
Psychological researchers
generally have less power over their subjects than their biomedical
counterparts. However, their experiments often occur within the context of a
university or other large institution, and invoke the more generalized prestige
of "science," which subjects may perceive as authoritative. For example, Milgram's famous studies[32]
testing the likelihood that people would obey the commands of a authority
figures, to the point of administering shocks to uncooperative subjects, took
place at Yale University and a New Haven research laboratory. Subjects would doubtless hold even higher
ethical expectations of a Christian institution. Consequently, the Christian
researcher might have greater moral authority and more effective powers of
persuasion with subjects from his or her religious tradition.
Informed Consent
Respect for person's demands
that, in most cases, subjects should enter into the research voluntarily and
with adequate information. They should receive the following information: the
research procedure; risks and anticipated benefits, if any; alternative
procedures (where therapy is involved); a statement inviting the subject to ask
questions; and an explicit statement allowing him or her to withdraw at any
time from the research. The details should be given in a way that the subject
can understand, allowing time for him or her to process the information and
return with additional questions. On occasion, it may be helpful to give some
sort of oral or written test of comprehension. (See the sample consent form in
Appendix B).
Deception
Many researchers argue that
the only way to obtain accurate information about certain aspects of their
subjects' behavior is not to inform them of the nature of the experiment or
study. If the subject knew his or her actions were being studied, this would
almost certainly result in the behavior being modified. Solutions to this problem have been
difficult to find. Simulations have proved inadequate, both methodologically
and ethically, since subjects who are asked to describe how they would react in
a particular situation may not know, or don't want to tell. On the other hand, if the simulations are
too realistic, they may cause stresses resembling those experienced by people
participating in the actual experiment.[33]
Deception appears to be
fairly widespread in various kinds of scientific research. In tests of
medicines and treatments, patientsand even doctorsare not told which persons
are in control groups or are receiving placebos. Likewise, deception occurs rather frequently in social science
studies. In 1978, Diener and Crandall estimate that between 19% and 44% of
social psychology and personality research involved direct lying, and one study
of psychology experiments suggested that complete information was given in only
3% of the cases reviewed.[34]
The category of subjects
being used may affect the prevalence of deceptive practices. Researchers may be
tempted to treat more cavalierly a disadvantaged group, feeling that they
cannot seriously disrupt the research, compared to more powerful or well-educated
subjects.
Deception always poses the
potential for harm to those being deceived, since they might have chosen to
avoid participation if they had been fully and accurately informed. They may
lose faith in the researcher and other authority figures, and even in the
merits of science in general.
When researchers deceive
subjects, their reputation for truthfulness may be harmed, as well as their
character. Scientists in general may
be less trusted if deceptive practices become widely known. And society may be
harmed, since deception could contribute to a general lack of trust and a
willingness by nonprofessionals to act deceptively themselves.
As researchers trick, deceive, and manipulate their subjects, they
thereby accustom themselves to denigrating other people's humanity. They may
also develop delusions of grandeur and omnipotence, and become progressively
more calloused and cynical, which could interfere with the integrity of their
scientific work.
Researchers need to rethink
the ethics, as well as the methodology, of some of the studies they propose,
since there will always be a delicate balance between the need to obtain
information that benefits humankind and the rights of individual subjects.[35]
In a Christian institution, research participants, as well as the constituency
and the general public, would doubtless expect a higher level of ethical
behavior than at a public institution.
Therefore, proposed research should be screened with care to ensure that
its methods and aims are consistent with the mission of the institution and the
moral principles of the denomination.
When planning an experiment,
the researcher should consider carefully the nature of the study to determine
whether deception is really necessary, and if so, then work out ways that
informed consent and subsequent debriefing can ameliorate the negative effects
of deception. Internal review boards
should look carefully at this issue and provide quite specific guidance to help
researchers resolve the problems through the application of ethical principles.
Privacy
Throughout the research
process, subjects' privacy should be carefully guarded. Social scientists
invade their subjects' privacy when they manipulate them into doing something
embarrassing or by disclosing private facts that place the subjects in a false
public light or intrude into their private domain. Having sensitive personal
information about a person means gaining power over that person, power that can
then be used to his or her detriment.
As a result, the person may be subjected to ridicule and intoleranceor
to legal or governmental action. His or
her community and descendants might even be harmed if the studies are used to
stereotype a particular ethnic group.
Personal interviews are
especially problematic, as the researcher's records on identified subjects may
be subpoenaed or used in legal proceedings against such persons, thereby
breaching the researcher's promise of confidentiality, putting the subjects at
risk, and possibly disrupting the researcher's work.[36] Therefore, invasion of privacy should be
undertaken only with greatest caution and after careful consideration of the
potential results.
The following questions are
central to assessing potential violations of privacy:
1. For what purpose(s) is the undocumented personal knowledge
sought?
2. Is this purpose a legitimate and important one?
3. Is the knowledge sought through invasion of privacy relevant
to its justifying purpose?
4. Is invasion of privacy the only or the least offensive means
of obtaining the information?
5. What restrictions or procedural restraints have been placed
on the privacy-invading techniques?
6. What protection is to be afforded the personal knowledge once
it has been acquired?[37]
With modern data processing
equipment, it has become much easier to run sophisticated analyses of
statistical information and identify the subjects of research, thereby
potentially endangering their welfare and privacy. Therefore, safeguards should
be taken to minimize any harm to subjects that might occureither by not
identifying them at all, or perhaps by using a number for each case, with the
code being destroyed after the analysis has been completed.
Recommendations
To help sensitize students
to the ethical and procedural dilemmas described above, Adventist colleges
should require ethics courses in each discipline. In addition, by reading about
current dilemmas in their fields of study, ethical issues involved in using
human subjects in research, and the codes of professional responsibility for
their professions,[38]
teachers can prepare themselves to discuss with their students the ways in
which Christian principles interact with real life.
Colleges should set up
institutional review boards to screen proposed research by both professors and
students. (See Appendix A) These boards
should develop guidelines defining the boundaries of acceptable research and
describing the appropriate steps to implement such studies. These guidelines
should be disseminated to students, staff, constituents, and other interested
persons.
No one should be allowed to undertake any type of research that
involves human subjects unless he or she has obtained prior clearance from the
IRB and has signed a form indicating understanding of and assent to the
principles governing such research. Some of the areas that should be addressed
in the guidelines and consent form include the following?
Ψ
Ethical and
scientific design of the experiment/study, including potential usefulness of
the research versus drawbacks
Ψ
Methods of data
collection and storage, including provisions for ensuring confidentiality of
data
Ψ
Methods of
choosing subjects
Ψ
Types of
subjects to be used (special cautions should be included when children, the
elderly, minorities, marginalized groups, persons engaged in illegal
activities, or prisoners are included as subjects of research; or when the
researcher-subject relationship might affect the ability to freely give
consent)
Ψ
Promises and
commitments made to subjects
Ψ
Informed
consent, including debriefing of subjects and permission for them to withdraw
at any time without repercussions.
Ψ
Other ethical
considerations (lying to subjects, asking subjects to engage in unethical
behavior, conflicts of interest, etc.)
Ψ
Any laws or
government guidelines that apply to the research being done.
Ψ
Method of
presenting the findings
By following the above
suggestions, Adventist colleges and universities will be able to help their
students discover the exciting mysteries of science, while respecting and
benefiting God's crowning creation, humankind.
Selected Bibliography
Abelson,
Raziel, and Marie-Louise Friquegnon. Ethics for Modern Life. New York:
St. martin's Press, 1991.
Beauchamp, Tom
L., et al. Ethical Issues in Social Science Research. Baltimore: Johns
Hopkins Unv. Press, 1982.
Braine, David,
and Harry Lesser, eds. Ethics, Technology, and Medicine. Aldershot:
Averbury, 1988.
Callahan, Joan
C., ed. Ethical Issues in Professional Life. New York: Oxford Univ. Press, 1988.
Christians,
Clifford G. and Catherine L. Covert, Teaching Ethics in Journalism
Education. Briarcliff Manor, N.V.:
The Hastings Center, 1980.
Curran,
Charles E. Issues in Sexual and Medical Ethics. Notre Dame: Univ. of
Notre Dame Press, 1978.
Durocher, Dbra
D. "Radiation Redux," American Journalism Review 16:2 (March
1994), p. 34.
Evans, J. D.
G., ed. Moral Philosophy and
Contemporary Problems. Cambridge: Cambridge Univ. Press, 1987.
Fletcher,
John. "Realities of Patient Consent to Medical Research," Hastings
Center Studies, 1:1 (1973), pp. 39-49.
Freud, Paul A.
Experimentation with Human Subjects. New York: George Braziller, 1970.
Fried,
Charles. Medical Experimentation: Personal Integrity and Social Policy.
Amsterdam: North-Holland Pub. Co., 1974.
Gorlin, Rena
A., ed. Codes of Professional Responsibility. Washington, D.C.: The
Bureau of National affairs, Inc., 1990.
Gray, Bradford
H. Human Subjects in Medical
Experimentation. New York: John Wiley & Sons, 1975.
Holmes, Helen
Bequaert, and Laura M. Purdy, eds. Feminist Perspectives in Medical Ethics.
Bloomington: Indiana Univ. Press, 1992.
Jones, James
H., Bad Blood: The Tuskegee Syphilis Experiment. New York: The Free
Press, 1981.
Katz, Jay. Experimentation
with Human Beings. New York: Russell Sage Foundation, 1972.
Milgram,
Stanley. Obedience to Authority. New York: Harper & Row, Pub. ,
1974.
Myers,
Christopher, "Medical Researchers Pressed to Use Female and Minority
Subjects, "The Chronicle of Higher Education XXXVII:43 (July 10,
1991).
National
Commission for the Protection of Human Subjects of Biomedical and Behavioral
Research, "The Belmont Report: Ethical Principles and Guidelines for the
Protection of Human Subjects in Research (OPRR Reports, April 18, 1979).
OPRR Reports,
"Protection of Human Subjects, "Code of Federal Regulations 45 CFR 46
(Revised March 8, 1983).
Pellegrino,
Edmund D., et al., eds. Ethics, Trust, and the Professions. Washington, D.C.: Georgetown Univ. Press,
1991.
President's
Commission for the Study of Ethical Problems in Medicine and biomedical and
Behavioral Research. "Implementing Human Research Regulations: The
Adequacy and Uniformity of Federal Rules and of Their Implementation."
Second Biennial Report, March 1983.
Public Health
Service, Department of Health and Human Services. "Answers to Frequently
Asked Questions." Rockville, Md., July 1984.
_______________. "FDA IRB Information Sheets"
(February 1989).
Rothman, David
J. Strangers at the Bedside: A History of How Law and Bioethics Transformed
Medical Decision Making. No City: Basic Books, 1991.
Sieber, Joan
E., ed. The Ethics of Social Research: Fieldwork, Regulation, and
Publication. New York: Springer-Verlag, 1982.
Sire, James. Discipleship of the Mind. Downers Grove,
ILL: InterVarsity Press, 1990.
Sjoberg,
Gidean, ed. Ethics, Politics, and Social Research. Cambridge, Mass.:
Schenkman Pub. Co. Inc., 1967.
Swyhart,
Barbara Ann DeMartino. Bioethical
Decision-Making. Philadelphia: Fortress Press, 1975.
White, Ellen
G. Education. Mountain View, CA: Pacific Press Pub. Assn., 1903.
Zillma, Dolf,
and Jennings Bryant, "Pornography, Sexual Callousness, and the
Trivialization of Rape," Journal of Communication 32:4 (Autumn
1982), p. 11.
Appendix A
"An IRB is a board, committee, or other group
formally designated by an institution to review, to approve the initiation of,
and to conduct continuing review of biomedical [or social science] research
involving human subjects in accordance with FDA regulations [and the policies
and principles of the institution]. An
IRB has the authority to approve, require modifications in (to secure
approval), or disapprove the research. The purpose of IRB review is to assure
that:
v
"Risks to
subjects are minimized: (1) by using procedures which are consistent with sound
research designed and which do not unnecessarily expose subjects to risk, and
(2) whenever appropriate, by using procedures already being performed on the
subjects for diagnostic or treatment purposes.
v
Risks are
reasonable in relation to anticipated benefits, if any, to subjects, and the
importance of the knowledge that may be expected to result.
v
Selection of
subjects is equitable.
v
Informed
consent will e sought from each prospective subject or the subject's legally
authorized representative and will be documented in accordance with, and to the
extend required, by FDA's informed consent regulation.
v
Where
appropriate, the research plan makes adequate provision for monitoring the data
collected to ensure the safety of subjects.
v
There are
adequate provisions to ensure the privacy of subjects and to maintain the
confidentiality of data.
v
Appropriate
additional safeguards have been included in the study to protect the rights and
welfare of subjects who are members of a particularly vulnerable group.[1]
Guidelines issued by the
U.S. Secretary of Health and Human Services in 1988 suggest the following
criteria for membership in an IRB:
1. Each IRB should include
at least five members from varying backgrounds; each member should be qualified
through experience and expertise. Members should be chosen so as to achieve a
balance in the areas of race, gender, and cultural background. They should also be selected for their
sensitivity to such issues as community attitudes. This will help ensure respect for the IRB's work, particularly in
terms of safeguarding the rights and welfare of human subjects. IRB members should be able to assess the
acceptability of proposed research in terms of institutional commitments and
regulations, applicable law, and standards of professional conduct and
practice. If the IRB regularly reviews
research that includes vulnerable groups such as children, prisoners, pregnant
women, or mentally handicapped persons, it should consider including one or
more persons who are knowledgeable about and experienced in working with these
types of individuals. (One person might fill two or more of the above listed
notes)
Other sources suggest including a lawyer, a statistician, a layperson,
or a ethicist who specializes in the field of the proposed research. It would
be helpful for the institution's media specialist to attend meetings as an
observer, so that he or she can answer questions from the administration or the
press regarding the type of research being done at the institution.
2. Every effort should be made to ensure that both sexes and members
of different professions are included in the IRB.
3. Each IRB should include at least one member whose primary
concerns lie in scientific areas, and at least one member whose concerns are
primarily in nonscientific areas.[2]
4. Members of the IRB should excuse themselves from voting on the
acceptability of any project that gives the appearance of a conflict of
interest with their work of their students.[3]
Further guidance in forming
an IRB, identifying research studies that require oversight, determining
frequency of meetings, reporting requirements, and other protocol involved in
its operation may be found in the endnote sources, and by consulting the administration
of one of the administration of one of the numerous universities where IRBs are
currently operating.
Prescott University
Advil, Oklahoma 78888
Instructions for Completing the Application for Review of Research
Using Human Subjects
Please answer
the following questions, taking care to make the answers intelligible to
non-specialists.
I. Provide a
brief description of the research you are proposing.
II. Subject selection:
a. Who will be the subjects?
How will you enlist their participation? If you plan to advertise for subjects,
include a copy of the ad.
b. Will the subjects be
selected for any specific characteristics, e.g., age, sex, race, ethnic origin,
religion, or any social or economic qualifications?
c. If the answer to
"b" is Yes, state why the selection is made on this basis. (If
children are used, be sure to include forms used to solicit the assent of the
children as well as of their parents or guardians.)
III. What will be done to the subjects? Explain
in detail your methods and procedures in this area. If you are using a mailed
questionnaire or handout, please include a copy.
IV. Are there any risks to subjects? If so, what
are these risks? Can the observations, if known outside the study, place the
subjects at risk of criminal or civil liability or be damaging to the subjects'
financial standing or employability? What potential benefits will accrue to
justify taking these risks?
V. Explain what you plan to do with the
research findings.
VI. Explain how your procedures will protect the
privacy of subjects and maintain confidentiality of identifiable information at
all phases of the research, i.e., when collecting, coding, storing, analyzing,
and disposing of the data.
VII. Does the research deal with sensitive issues
such as illegal conduct, drug use, sexual behavior, or use of alcohol?
VII. State
specifically what information will be provided to the subjects about the
investigation. Is any of this information deceptive? State how the subjects'
informed consent will be obtained. Attach the form that will be used when
written consent is required.
IX. Describe the debriefing and follow-up
process you will use to explain the experiment and answer questions after the
research is completed.
X. Will electrical or mechanical equipment be
used? If yes, has it been checked for safety?
Please include 10 copies of
this form if the application if the entire IRB will be reviewing your application. Be sure to include all relevant supporting
documents include consent forms, letters sent to recruit participants,
questionnaires to be completed by participants, routing form for sponsored
projects, and any other materials germane to human subjects review.
XI. Other obligations: (a) Report to the
Institutional Review Board any unanticipated effects harmful to subjects that
become apparent during the research. (b) Cooperate with the continuing review
of this research project by submitting annual reports and a final report. (c)
Maintain all documents required by the institution and federal guidelines.
(This
sample form merges guidelines from SDA and public institutions. In the U.S., certain types of research are
exempt from more intense scrutiny. See federal statutes or local university
regulations for category definitions.)
NOTES AND REFERENCES
[1] Department of Health and Human Services, "Answers to Frequently Asked Questions," FDA IRB Information Sheets (Rockville, Md.: Public Health Service, FDA, February 1989), pp. 23,24; see also President's Commission for the Study of Ethical Problems in Medicine and biomedical and Behavioral Research, "Implementing Human Research Regulations, Second Biennial Report on the Adequacy and Uniformity of Federal Review Rules and Policies, and Their Implementation, for the Protection of Human Subjects" (March 1983).
[2] "Institutional Review Boards, "Proposed Rules, Part 56, Section 56.107, Federal Register 53:218 (November 10, 1988).
[1]
"Bericht όber Eine bei Kindern beobachtete
Endemie infectiφser Kolpitis," Virchow's Archiv, Bd 99, Heft 2
(1885), ppl 263,264; cited in Jay Katz, Experimentation With Human Beings
(New York: Russell Sage Foundation, 1972), p. 285.
[2]
M. H. Pappwoth, Human Guinea Pigs
(Boston: n.p., 1967), p. 61.
[3]
James H. Jones, Bad Blood: The Tuskegee Syphilis
Experiment (New York: The Free Press, 1981).
[4]
Henry K. Beecher, "Ethics and Clinical
Research, "New England Journal of Medicine 74 (1966), cited in
David J. Rothman, Strangers at the Bedside: A History of How Law and Bioethics
Transformed Medical Decision making (No City: Basic Books, 1991), p. 16.
[5]
Reported by Sue V. Rosser, "Revisioning
Clinical Research: Gender and the Ethics of Experimental Design, " in
Helen Bequaert Holmes and Laura M. Rurdy, eds., Feminist Perspectives in
Medical Ethics (Bloomington: Indiana University Press, 1992), p. 131.
[6]
After a two-year investigation, Massachusetts
Rep. Edward Markey in 1986 released a report detailing the experiments,
entitled "American Nuclear Guinea Pigs: Three Decades of Radioactive
Experiments on U.S. Citizens." Markey stated that officials had conducted
"repugnant" and "bizarre" experiments on hospital patients,
prison inmates, and hundreds of others who "might not have retained their
full faculties for informed consent" (reported by Debra D. Durocher in
"Radiation Redux, "American Journalism Review 16:2[March
1994], p. 35).
[7]
Dolf Zillman and Jennings Bryant,
"Pornography, Sexual Callousness, and the Trivialization of Rape,"
Journal of Communication 32:4 (Autumn 1982), p. 11.
[8]
Dorothy Nelkin, "Forbidden Research: Limits
to Inquiry in the Social Sciences, "in Tom L. Beauchamp, et al, Ethical
Issues in Social Science Research (Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University
Press, 1982), p. 168.
[9]
L. Festinger, H. W. Riecken, and S. Schachter,
When Prophecy Fails (Minneapolis: University of Minneapolis Press, 1956),
reported by Joan Cassell, "Harms, Benefits, Wrongs, and Rights in
Fieldwork," in Joan E. Sieber, ed., The Ethics of Social Research (New
York: Springer-Verlag, 1982), p. 25.
[10]
Nelkin, in Beauchamp, p. 166.
[11]
Paul E. Freund, Experimentation With Human
Subjects (New York: George Braziller, 1970), p. xiii.
[12]
Nelkin, in Beauchamp, et al, p. 163.
[13]
Paul Ramsey, cited by Rothman, p. 96.
[14]
Beecher, cited by Rothman, pp. 82,83.
[15]
Rothman, pp. 3,10
[16]
The whole text of the code reads as follows:
"Under no circumstances is a doctor permitted to do anything that would
weaken the physical or mental resistance of human being except form strictly
therapeutic or prophylactic indications imposed in the interest of his
patient" (cited by Charles Curran, Issues in Sexual and Medical Ethics [Notre
Dame: University of Notre Dame Press, 1978], p. 77.
[17]
Eugene Fontinell, "Contraception and Ethics
of Relationships," in What Modern Catholics Believe About Birth
Control, cited in Barbara Ann DeMartino Swyhart, Bioethical
Decision-making (Philadelphia: Fortress Press, 1975), p. 49.
[18]
Robert C. Mortinmer, "The Standard of Moral
Right and Wrong," in Raziel Abelson and Marie-Louise Friquegnon, Ethics
for Modern Life (New York: St. Martin's Press, 1991), p. 20.
[19]
Henry Stob, Ethical Reflections: Essays on
Moral Themes (Grand Rapids, MI: William B. Eerdmans Pub. Co., 1978), p.
130.
[20]
Ellen G. White, Education (Mountain View,
CA: Pacific Press Pub. Assn., 1903), p. 17.
[21] Curran, p. 87
[22].Hans Jonas, "Philosophical Reflections on Experimenting with Human Subjects," in Experimentation with Human Subjects, pp. 18-22; cited by Curran, p. 87.
[23] Curran, p. 89.
[24] The National Commission for the Protection of Human Subjects of Biomedical and Behavioral Research, "The Belmont Report: Ethical Principles and Guidelines for the Protection of Human Subjects in Research" (OPRR Reports, April 18, 1979), p. 4.
[25] Mortimer in Ableson and Friquegnon, p. 15.
[26] Personal communication from John Wesley Taylor, V, Ph.D., Director of University Research, Montemorelos University, Mexico, February 24, 1994.
[27] David D. Rutstein, "The Ethical Design of Human Experiments," in Freund, p. 363.
[28] Charles Fried, Medical Experimentation: Personal Integrity and social Policy (Amsterdam: North-Holland Pub. Co., 1974), pp. 25,27
[29] Ibid., p. 42.
[30] Mortimer, in Abelson and Friquegnon, p. 19.
[31] John Fletcher, "Realities of Patient Consent to Medical Research, "Hastings Center Studies, 1:1 (1973), pp. 39-49.
[32] Stanley Milgram, Obedience to Authority: An
Experimental View (New York: Harper and Row, Pub., 1974).
[33] Alan C. Elms, "Keeping Deception Honest: Justifying Conditions for Social Scientific Research Stratagems," in Beauchamp, et al, pp. 235,236.
[34] Edward Diener and Rick Grandall, Ethics in Social and Behavioral Research (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1978), cited in Beauchamp, et al, p. 169.
[35] If deception is considered absolutely essential to the experiment, one researcher suggests using it only "when (1) there is no other feasible way to obtain the desired information, (2) the likely benefits substantially outweigh the likely harms, (3) subjects are given the option to withdraw from participation at any time without penalty, (4) any physical or psychological harm to subjects is temporary, (5) subjects are d3ebriefed as to all substantial deception (Margaret Mead, "Research With Human Beings: Model Derived From Anthropological Field Practice," in Freund, p. 167).
[36] R. Jay Wallace, Jr., "Privacy and the Use of Data in Epidemiology," in Beauchamp, p. 297.
[37] W. A. Parent, "Privacy, Morality, and the Law," in Joan C. Callahan, ed., Ethical Issues in Professional Life (New York: Oxford University Press, 1988), p. 220.